[Printable (pdf) version of this lesson]
an
to know (of people); to be acquainted with (of a place or a topic)
bel
to take; to bring
dó–
Prefix (verb): to cause to VERB
il
to pay attention
láad
to perceive
lothel
to know (of information)
naya
to care for
nin
to cause; to be causal
thel
to get; to obtain
thi
to have
It seems odd to the English ear, but the verb “bel” means both “to take” and “to bring.” English uses a pair of unrelated verbs in what linguists call a “deictic pair;” not all languages do this to the same extent that English does. An example may be helpful in reducing the oddness: when we say “Anna brought a casserole to the pot-luck,” or “Anna took a casserole to the pot-luck,” the same person transported the same food to the same event; only the point of view of the speaker (linguist-speak: the deixis) has changed—either the speaker was at the party or she was elsewhere, respectively. Which English verb should be used in translating “bel” can (but need not necessarily) be made clear (linguist-speak: the deixis can be disambiguated) by the context in which it occurs.
The prefix “dó–” (cause to VERB) added to a “stative verb” (a verb that discusses a state of being—one that, in English, would be an adjective), turns that verb into an “active verb” (one that discusses an action—that, in English, would be a verb). For example, “míi” means “to be amazed;” “dómíi” means “to cause to be amazed” or, in more colloquial English, “to amaze.” The other effect of this transformation is that the Subject of “míi” (the one who is amazed) becomes the Object of “dómíi” (the one whom someone or something else causes to be amazed) and the one doing the amazing is the new Subject.
The verbs “láad” and “il” are essentially Láadan. “Láad” means “to perceive by means of the senses.” “Il” means “to pay attention by means of the senses.” An English example might clarify the distinction between perception and attention. Using the visual sensory modality, in English we “see” a picture (perception) or we “look at” it (attention). Using the auditory sensory modality, we might “hear” music (perception) or “listen to” it (attention). How to specify the particular sense modality will be made clear in the lesson on the Instrument Case. In the meantime, it is perfectly proper Láadan to use “láad” and “il” without a sensory modality, in which case they would be translated as simply “to perceive” and “to pay attention,” respectively.
“Láad” (to perceive) is, of course, the other half of the name of this language: “Láadan” [láad (perceive) + dan (language)].
In the above, the new abbreviation VP means “Verb Phrase;” it stands for the optional Auxiliary + the Verb, the Verb Complex, or the Ø-Verb (in the Identifier structure) + the optional Negative. We’ll be using this new abbreviation henceforward.
–th
Suffix (noun phrase): Object Case
To mark a Case Phrase as an Object, add
Along with the Object Case, we introduce the conjunction “úthú” (what). Though, in English, it resembles a question-word, this conjunction introduces a clause (in Láadan, a “clause” is defined as a Verb Phrase with its Case Phrase(s)—like a sentence but with neither Type-of-Sentence Word nor Evidence Word) that fills the role of an Object in the sentence. It translates as the “what” in the English sentence, “I know what you did last summer.”
The person teaches peace (I’m told).
Does the person teach peace?
Who teaches peace?
What does the person teach?
Note that the Object occurs after the Subject. Word order in Láadan is much less flexible than that in English.
Note the “e” added between “shon” and
Note the translation of “bebáath.” It must be “what” rather than “whom” because verbs involving communication take as their Object the thing communicated. The person to whom the Object is communicated involves a different Case which will be presented in a future lesson.
The cat doesn’t want the grain (hypothetically).
Doesn’t the cat want the grain?
Who/what doesn’t want the grain?
What doesn’t the cat want?
Bíi meden hoshem Méri betha leth wa.
Mary’s grandchildren help me.
Báa meden hoshem Méri betha leth?
Do Mary’s parents help me?
Who (few/several) help me?
Báa meden hoshem bebáatha leth?
Whose grandchildren help me?
Báa meden hoshem Méri betha bebáath?
Whom do Mary’s parents help?
I am amazed.
The person causes me to be amazed.
(The person amazes me.)
The people amaze me.
Whom does Margaret help?
Margaret helps me.
Who helps Margaret?
I help Margaret.
I help her/him.
Note that personal names do not receive Case endings. When the name of a person or animal fulfills the role of a Case that would take a suffix, the name is followed by the pronoun “be” which accepts the Case ending instead. This rule applies to the names of living and once-living persons and creatures but not to the names of places or times.
Sometimes surface similarities can occur in any language. Notice that “beth” in the examples above is “be” (third person singular pronoun) +
In the English sentence, “I know what you did last summer,” the word “what” does quite a bit of heavy lifting. The sentence says “You did a thing last summer, and I know the nature of that thing.” But, rather than stringing it out as a compound sentence, it embeds the clause “you did [a thing] last summer” inside the sentence, “I know ….” The “what” stands as the Object of the embedded sentence, and the embedded sentence is the Object of the outer sentence. In Láadan, “úthú” does this heavy lifting:
Bíi eril radom ne úthú di Mázhareth wa.
You forgot what Margaret said.
Bíi ril thi le úthú néde Ána wa.
I have what Anna wants.
Báa aril bel behizh úthú ril thi ebalá wáa.
She will bring what the baker has.
Does the bird want fruit?
Does your bird want fruit?
Does the bird want my fruit?
Báa néde babí netho yu lethoth?
Does your bird want my fruit?
The Possessive becomes part of the Case Phrase. Since we’re treating the Possessive Noun Phrase as a single unit (and so nothing can be inserted between the elements of the unit), the Case Suffix must be added to the end—on the Possessor, not the thing possessed. In the first example above, “yu” (fruit) is the Object: “yuth”. When we change the Object to “my fruit” (yu letho), the Object suffix is attached to the end of the phrase: “yu lethoth”.
Of course, the Case suffix for the Subject is what is known as a “null suffix” or “zero suffix” (or, in Linguist Speak, the suffix is said to “present a null surface form”). That “null suffix” on a Possessive Subject is attached to the end of the Possessive sequence; however, since it’s a “null suffix” (which cannot be seen or heard), we haven’t had to deal with it before now. Perhaps it’ll be a little clearer if presented in morphemic analysis of the first and fourth examples above:
Báa
Q
néde
Want
babí
Bird + SUBJ
yuth?
Fruit + OBJ
Báa
Q
néde
Want
babí
netho
Bird
You1 + POSS + SUBJ
yu
lethoth?
Fruit
I + POSS + OBJ
I had the clothes.
I had this clothing.
Bíi eril thi le budeth beye wa.
I had some clothing.
Bíi eril thi le bud amedarahátheth wa.
I had the dancer’s (chance) clothing.
Bíi eril thi le bud edinethe amedaraháthath wa.
I had the dancer’s (birth) cousin’s (chance) clothing.
A multiply-possessive structure, such as that above, is still a single unit, and so the Object suffix belongs at the end of the Possessive structure.
In contrast to the Possessive structure, when using the Demostrative pronouns (hi-forms) or Indefinite pronouns (beye-forms) as modifying postposition, the case suffix remains on the noun being modified—as seen in the second and third examples above.
They (few) want this/that.
They want the same thing.
They want something.
They want nothing.
They want anything but grain.
Bíi menéde bezh hizh hizheth wa.
They want each other.
Notice the pair of examples using “rath”
1
Bíi aril rathi worabalin wowithid woth wi.
2
Báa eril thel shem Elízhabeth betha woloyo wohéezheth?
3
Báa nime doth berídan ábedátha háamudath?
4
Mehan háawith sherídanizh bebáathath?
5
Bíi yod ra wodo wohomid Máyel betho wolaya wohedeth wo.
6
Bíi eril dibáa omá úthú them hothul Therísha betha wáa.
Note the word “rathi” in #1. As a straightforward opposite of “thi” (to have), “rathi” means “to lack”
Note that there is no Object suffix on “woth” in #1 above. This is because there can be no ambiguity. Because “wisdom” cannot “want” anything, in Láadan its Object suffix is optional; you may use the suffix or not, at your discretion—so long as the meaning is not ambiguous.
The same applies to #8 below: Láadan cannot “speak the parents,” so no Object suffix is required. And to #13 below. And also to #5 above and #15 and #17 below, although in these examples we have chosen to use the Object suffix; it remains grammatically correct even when it is not required.
Note that there is no Type-of-Sentence Word in #4. Because the sentence contains a form of the Interrogatory Pronoun, “bebáa,” there can be no confusion that this is a question. “Báa” at the beginning of the sentence would not be wrong, but it isn’t required in a less-formal setting.
7
zháamid
8
Bíi aríli medi thul halátha wáa.
Láadan
9
melothel ra bezh
10
wam
11
delith lanetha betho
12
onida bebáatha
In & #9, remember that we can specify the life-stage of any life form using the set of life-stage prefixes; in #7
In #9, in order to incorporate the clause in the second column as the Object of the sentence in the first, we’ll need to use “úthú” (CONJobj).
13
We (several) close the door.
14
Did the storm cause what you-few now have to clean?
15
The beautiful dancers brought the extremely hot drink?
16
Suzette will cause Margaret’s sister to laugh (obviously).
17
Does age cause the needleworker to be weary?
18
The high mountain does not remember the dancing clouds.
The verb “close” in #13 might be a bit of a challenge. We know the verb “rahu” (to be closed); “to close” can also be termed “to cause to be closed.” “Dórahu” [from
The verb “rahu” (to be closed) is a stative verb (a verb that, in English, would be an adjective). Formally, it takes only a Subject. Less formally, the assignment of an Object to a Stative Verb implies a conversion to a Transitive Active Verb (one that would, in English, be a Verb that accepts an Object). No confusion will ensue, promise! So, a less-formal usage would find “rahu” to be an acceptable form to mean “to close.”
A approach similar to “dórahu” in #13 should be taken in #16 and #17, but here it is more straightforward because the syntax “cause to VERB” is clearly present in the English. Don’t be confused by the English tendency to put the Object between “cause” and “to VERB;” Láadan uses the prefix
The verb “clean” in #14 might be challenging; like “close” the formal word for “to clean” is “dóhéthe”
Note the word “dóhada” (cause to laugh) as seen in #16. It has another meaning: “to be comical” or “to be funny.” When used in this way, it will take no Object—that is, the Subject is generally “funny” or “comical;” there is no specific person being “caused to laugh.” This is another example of the porosity (sometimes, unlike here, only informally) of the boundary between stative and active verbs (those that would, in English, be adjectives and verbs, respectively).
Did the word “age” in #17 give you pause? Any verb can be used as a noun, so long as the result is not nonsense. For “stative verbs”, the translation of the nominal form is perhaps most easily formed by adding
1
The young man will lack wisdom, of course.
2
Did Elizabeth’s offspring get a black goat?
3
Is the farmer’s aunt willing to follow the juvenile pig?
4
Whose niece do the children know?
5
Michael’s strong horse doesn’t eat red grain (I guess).
6
The teacher asked what Teresa’s grandmother needed.
7
Bethany cared.
Bíi eril naya Bétheni zháamideth wa.
Bethany cared for the senior-animal.
8
A worker’s parents will, in some far time, speak.
Bíi aríli medi thul halátha Láadan wáa.
In some far-off future, a worker’s parents will speak Láadan.
9
Do the fledglings fear (are the fledglings afraid)?
Báa mehéeya yáababí úthú melothel ra bezh?
Do the fledglings fear what they don’t know (are the fledglings afraid of what they don’t know)?
10
Stones teach.
Stones teach stillness.
11
Will Marsha pay attention?
Báa aril il Másha delith lanetha bethoth?
Will Marsha pay attention to her friend’s hair?
12
Did the aliens perceive, long ago?
Báa eríli meláad néehá onida bebáathath?
Whose family did the aliens perceive, long ago?
13
14
Báa nin rohoro úthú ril medush medóhéthe nezh?
15
Bíi eril mebel mewoháya wohamedarahá wohowahul worana wa.
16
Bíi dóhada Shuzhéth hena Máthu bethath wi.
17
18
top